Journal of Threatened Taxa | www.threatenedtaxa.org | 26 August 2021 | 13(9): 19246–19253

 

 

ISSN 0974-7907 (Online) | ISSN 0974-7893 (Print) 

https://doi.org/10.11609/jott.7510.13.9.19246-19253

#7510 | Received 09 June 2021 | Final received 28 July 2021 | Finally accepted 09 July 2021

 

 

First record of interspecies grooming between Raffles’ Banded Langur and Long-tailed Macaque

 

Zan Hui Lee 1, Andie Ang 2 & Nadine Ruppert 3

 

1,2,3 School of Biological Sciences, Universiti Sains Malaysia, 11800 USM, Penang, Malaysia.

1,2,3 Malaysian Primatological Society, No 14, Lorong Nuri 3/1, Taman Nuri, 09000, Kulim, Kedah, Malaysia.

2 Raffles’ Banded Langur Working Group, Mandai Nature

2 IUCN SSC Primate Specialist Group (Asia)

3 IUCN SSC Primate Specialist Group (Section Small Apes)

1 zanhui96@gmail.com, 2 andie.ang@colorado.edu, 3 n.ruppert@usm.my (corresponding author)

 

 

 

Abstract: In primates, observations of interspecies grooming are not uncommon, especially between species of the same genus. However, little is reported about grooming between different genera and less is discussed about its ecological significance. Here, we report the first sighting of Long-tailed Macaques grooming the Critically Endangered and rare Raffles’ Banded Langur during two independent events at Gunung Lambak Recreational Forest in Kluang, Malaysia.

 

Keywords: Interspecies interactions, Macaca fascicularis, Malaysia, Presbytis femoralis, primate, Singapore.

 

 

 

Editor: Mewa Singh, University of Mysore, Mysuru, India.         Date of publication: 26 August 2021 (online & print)

 

Citation: Lee, Z.H., A. Ang & N. Ruppert (2021). First record of interspecies grooming between Raffles’ Banded Langur and Long-tailed Macaque. Journal of Threatened Taxa 13(9): 19246–19253. https://doi.org/10.11609/jott.7510.13.9.19246-19253

 

Copyright: © Lee et al. 2021. Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License.  JoTT allows unrestricted use, reproduction, and distribution of this article in any medium by providing adequate credit to the author(s) and the source of publication.

 

Funding: World Wide Fund for Nature (Singapore and Malaysia) disbursed through the Malaysian Primatological Society, Universiti Sains Malaysia Gra-ASSIST scheme, and Mandai Nature.

 

Competing interests: The authors declare no competing interests.

 

Author details: Lee Zan Hui is a postgraduate student in Zoology at Universiti Sains Malaysia who focuses on primate ecology and behaviour. He is a trained environmental scientist with interest in spatial ecology and conservation of wildlife and their habitats.  Andie Ang is a primatologist studying Asian colobines and chairing the RafflesBanded Langurs Working Group, Singapore. She is a research scientist with Mandai Nature and a visiting researcher to School of Biological Sciences, Universiti Sains Malaysia and member of the IUCN SSC Primate Specialist Group (Asia). Nadine Ruppert is a zoologist focusing on primates, human-wildlife interactions, plant-animal interactions, and conservation issues related to Malaysia. She is a senior lecturer at Universiti Sains Malaysia, vice president of the Malaysian Primatological Society and member of the IUCN SSC Primate Specialist Group Section on Small Apes.

 

Author contributions: conceptualisation—NR; field work and data analysis—ZHL; first manuscript draft—ZHL; manuscript corrections—NR, AA; advisory role—AA.

 

Acknowledgements: We are grateful to the Department of Wildlife and National Parks Peninsular Malaysia for granting the research permit (P-00183-15-19 issued to NR) to study the behaviour of Raffles’ Banded Langurs at Gunung Lambak and to the funders of this study: World Wide Fund (Singapore and Malaysia) disbursed through the Malaysian Primatological Society, Universiti Sains Malaysia Gra-ASSIST scheme, and Mandai Nature.

 

 

introduction

 

Most primate species are highly social, and behaviour like allogrooming, alloparenting, and playing between members of the same species are regularly observed in langurs and macaques (Lehman et al. 2007; Matsuda et al. 2015). Social grooming is a key behaviour in primates to facilitate kinship reciprocity (Schino & Aureli 2010), affiliation (Dunbar 1991; Lehman et al. 2007), and hygiene (Goosen 1981), involving at least two actors (groomer and recipient) physically touching each other. As an affiliative interaction, grooming plays a significant role in primate societies to reinforce social bonds and networks (Dunbar 2011).  Grooming also facilitates communication, contributing to the development of social cohesion (Dunbar 2011; Dunbar & Lehman 2013; Grueter et al. 2013). Within social groups of macaques, post-conflict reconciliation and consolation often involve grooming (Long-tailed Macaques Macaca fascicularis: Cords 1992; Stump-tailed Macaques M. arctoides: Call et al. 2002; Barbary Macaques M. sylvanus: McFarland & Majolo 2011). Social grooming has a direct effect on the physiological health of individuals, and other positive effects are associated with release of pleasure hormone (i.e., oxytocin and endorphin; Dunbar 2010) or suppression of stress hormones (i.e., cortisol: Wooddell et al. 2016). Seyfarth (1977)  proposed that social grooming in female Cercopithecidae is principally directed toward high-ranking individuals (reviewed in Schino 2001). This rank-related reciprocity in allogrooming has been demonstrated for 16 species and nine genera (Alouatta, Cercopithecus, Cebus, Erythrocebus, Lemur, Macaca, Miopithecus, Sapajus and Semnopithecus) (Schino & Aureli 2008a; Tiddi et al. 2012). Evidence of trading allogrooming for alloparenting is also found in female Long-tailed Macaques (Gumert 2007).

Asymmetries in allogrooming resemble the context of ecological economy as a trade for other commodities including food (reviewed in Barrett & Henzi 2001; see Russell & Phelps 2013) and services (Lazaro-Perea et al. 2004). Several studies have identified asymmetrical cost and benefits between groomer and recipient, considering that altruistic behaviour benefits the recipient but imposes some cost to the donor (Schino & Aureli 2008b; Russell & Phelps 2013). Grooming time is considered as cost to the groomer in their daily activity budgets as metabolic cost and loss of foraging opportunities (Russell & Phelps 2013). Cost and benefits can also be categorised into immediate and delayed effects. For example, ectoparasite removal is an immediate hygiene benefit to the recipient and constitutes a food source for the groomer but has a delayed risk of disease transmission from the close contact and ectoparasite consumption (reviewed in Veá et al. 1999; Russell & Phelps 2013). Furthermore, there is an immediate cost of time spent on grooming in return of immediate tension reduction with delayed agonistic support through social bonding (reviewed in Veá et al. 1999; Russell & Phelps 2013).

Grooming is not restricted to members of the same species but has been documented in members of different species in the wild such as between Rhesus Macaque M. mulatta and Sambar Deer Rusa unicolor (Vasava & Mahato 2013), Rhesus Macaque and Hanuman Langur Semnopithecus entellus (Nerlekar 2012), and Red-tailed Monkey Cercopithecus ascanius and Blue Monkey C. mitis (Gathua 2000). Interspecies allogrooming has also been seen in captivity between Long-tailed Macaque and Patas Monkey Erythrocebus patas (Baker & Preston 1973), capuchin monkeys Cebus albifrons & Sapajus apella and spider monkeys Ateles geoffroyi and A. paniscus (Maple & Westlund 1975). However, most available studies were only reviewed within the context of intraspecies allogrooming. Anecdotal records of interspecies allogrooming have been reported from the field, usually for species of the same genus and/or within mixed species groups with hybrids (e.g., M. nemestrina x M. fascicularis at Sepilok, Sabah; Gilhooly & Colquhoun 2018) but, to the authors’ knowledge, published observations of intergeneric grooming in wild sympatric primates are not available. Interspecies grooming between primates of different genera is rarely reported from the wild, and has, to our knowledge, not been documented for the Critically Endangered (Ang et al. 2020; status currently being revised, A. Ang, pers. comm. 01.vi.2021) and rare Raffles’ Banded Langur Presbytis femoralis.

Here, we report the first sighting of Long-tailed Macaques grooming Raffles’ Banded Langurs in Gunung Lambak Recreational Forest in Johor, Malaysia, and discuss the potential meaning of this interspecies interaction.

 

 

Methods

 

The state of Johor in Peninsular Malaysia is home to six species of non-human primates, namely: Sunda Slow Loris Nycticebus coucang, Long-tailed Macaque Macaca f. fascicularis, Southern Pig-tailed Macaque M. nemestrina, Reid’s Dusky Langur Trachypithecus o. obscurus, Raffles’ Banded Langur Presbytis femoralis, and Malaysian White-handed Gibbon Hybolates l. lar. Specifically, the Raffles’ Banded Langur is Critically Endangered (Ang et al. 2020) due to its small population size and restricted distribution in fragmented habitats. This species is only found in southern Peninsular Malaysia (states of Johor and Pahang) and the Republic of Singapore with an estimated global population size of fewer than 400 individuals (Ang et al. 2020).

Gunung Lambak is a twin-peak hill (highest at 510 m) located in Kluang, Johor (2.0275° N, 103.3575° E). Also known as Gunung Lambak Recreational Forest, it has an area of 744 ha (i.e., Renggam Forest Reserve) (Image 1). The vegetation consists mainly of secondary forest with patches of old growth trees. Pioneer tree species, such as Campnosperma auriculatum, Macaranga spp. and non-native Acacia spp. are common at the foot of the recreation forest and along the trails (Lee Zan Hui pers. obs. 14.iv.2021). Despite being an isolated secondary forest, trees of the Fagaceae family, Lithocarpus cantelyanus, L. sundaicus, Castanopsis acuminatissima, C. scortechinii, and C. inermis have been observed fruiting at different times throughout the year providing a food source to the primate community at the site (Lee Zan Hui pers. obs. 14.iv.2021). All six species of primates in Johor can be found in Gunung Lambak. Renggam Forest Reserve also holds a high in situ conservation value for endangered Dipterocarpaceae species, with Hopea glaucescens and H. johorensis being listed as Critically Endangered, Dipterocarpus sublamellatus as Endangered, and Shorea exelliptica, S. gibbose, S. gratissima, H. nutans, and Anisoptera megistocarpa as Vulnerable (Chua et al. 2010).

Non-invasive opportunistic observations were made on a group of Raffles’ Banded langurs (hereafter RBL) during the assessment of phenology transects for the study of the feeding ecology of this species. Footage of behaviour was recorded with a DSLR camera (Nikon D5600) with a telephoto lens (Nikkor 200–500 mm). The study group consists of 11 individuals (one adult male, five adult females with three dependent infants, and two juvenile females). Long-tailed Macaques (LTMs), which are observed in groups of up to 20 individuals in the area, range sympatrically with RBLs and can often be observed in close proximity to RBLs.

 

Results

 

The first interspecies grooming event was observed in the late afternoon of 02 October 2020 at the foot of Gunung Lambak during light drizzling rain. At 1715h, a group of ca. 20 LTMs were attracted to food provided by people with the intention to feed macaques at the roadside. Intragroup aggression among LTMs competing for the provisioned food was observed and some LTMs climbed up the trees nearby where the focal RBL troop of 11 individuals was resting, likely trying to avoid direct confrontation with dominant LTMs who guarded the food. At 1730h, RBLs moved from the forest toward the roadside, preparing to cross the road to their sleeping sites. At 1733h, two LTMs, who both appeared to be subadult males, approached an adult female RBL and groomed her (Image 2, Video 1). The grooming event was unidirectional and only LTMs engaged in grooming while the RBL female was solely receiving. This RBL was a lactating mother who had a dependent infant (which was out of sight during the grooming event). The rest of the RBLs who were not engaged with the LTMs were resting and feeding on flower buds and leaves of Garcinia mangostana, Acacia mangium, and other trees nearby. One of the two LTM groomers stopped grooming and moved onto a higher branch after ca. half a minute while the other LTM continued grooming for eight minutes. This LTM was observed picking substances from the RBL’s fur and feeding on it. Without leaving its location, the LTM also occasionally fed on flower buds of the tree (species unidentified) that they sat on. No aggression was observed in this interspecies interaction. The female RBL was observed being vigilant, actively scanning the surrounding environment and responding to vehicles that passed by. At 1753h, the grooming event was interrupted by the alpha RBL male as he approached the female and displaced the LTM, before directing the RBL group to cross the road. The LTM group, including the two groomers, followed the RBLs to cross the road. Both species separated after the road crossing and headed to their respective sleeping sites.

The second observation of interspecies grooming was on 08 October 2020, around 1808h during clear, sunny weather. A female subadult LTM was observed actively seeking RBLs to groom while the RBL group was feeding on sprouting leaf buds of a tall Saga Tree Adenanthera pavonina at the same site (Image 3, Video 2). The RBL group was observed foraging on leaf buds, which were limited and sparsely distributed across the crown of the Saga Tree, then the LTM groomer approached for grooming. In contrast to the first observation with a fairly long grooming bout (around eight minutes) between the fixed groomer and recipient pair, RBLs actively terminated this grooming event by leaving the position for foraging, hence leaving the LTM groomer to seek another recipient. Having been rejected by some RBLs, the LTM female seemed cautious about approaching RBLs, especially female adults. She started by touching a RBL’s tail from a distance with an extended hand, and only if the RBL stayed in position, she got closer to inspect (possibly for ectoparasites), also presenting her body in front of the RBL recipient. At least two RBL juvenile females and two adult females were eventually groomed at different timings by the same LTM (Table 1). The LTM groomer was also observed extracting substances from RBL’s fur by hand-picking and feeding on it as well as directly biting the RBL fur with her mouth. Although no direct confrontation or aggressive behaviour was observed, some RBLs appeared impatient towards the groomer and prioritised feeding on the leaf buds. While being groomed, the RBLs seemed to pay high attention on locating the scarce food resources instead of being vigilant to the surroundings. It could not be observed whether the other individuals nearby and/ or the alpha male of the RBL group were engaged in the event or groomed by the LTM at any time but the troop was directed by his call to leave the Saga Tree and headed to the sleeping site by 1820h, eventually ending the grooming event as the LTM groomer was left behind.

 

 

Discussion

 

Interspecies grooming between RBL and other primate species has not been reported before. In Singapore, where the two species are also found sympatrically, grooming has been observed only once between the two species during more than four years of continuous research (A. Ang pers. obs. 13.ii.2021). However, interspecies grooming between Dusky Langurs T. obscurus and LTMs has frequently been observed between different individuals in mainland Penang, Malaysia with a mixed-species group reported from Cherok Tokun (Nadine Ruppert pers. obs. 2019–2020), and Dusky Langurs, LTMs and White-thighed Langurs Presbytis siamensis form tolerant foraging associations with juveniles being observed playing together near a residential area in Ampang Jaya, Selangor, Malaysia (N. Ruppert pers. obs. 08.ii.2020). Published information on interspecies grooming, with clear explanations, is often from studies conducted in captivity, such as zoos with mixed species displays, e.g., capuchin monkeys and spider monkeys grooming each other (Maple & Westlund 1975), or laboratory settings designed to test hypotheses in interspecies interactions, e.g., interspecies infant interactions between LTMs and Patas Monkeys (Baker & Preston 1973). There are only a handful of publications on interspecies grooming in the wild, such as the unidirectional grooming events by macaques toward non-primate species such as deers, mostly occurring during social coalition when the ungulates followed a primate troop and foraged on fallen food items (Tsuji et al. 2007; Vasava & Mahato 2013).

The reported interspecies grooming here was unidirectional in both observations, with young LTMs of both sexes being the groomers. As adult RBLs have larger body sizes (weight around 6 kg, 59 cm in body length and up to 84 cm with tail length), ca. two times the size of young LTMs (Ang et al. 2016), the LTMs likely groomed for rank-related benefits (Schino & Aureli 2008b), especially in the first observation where a macaque intragroup conflict was observed just before the grooming event when the young macaques were displaced from the food provisioning site. Post-conflict consolation is an affiliative interaction from the victim of aggression and individuals other than the former aggressor (Aureli 1992). Bystanders, who are not directly involved in the aggression may offer affiliative interactions to relieve stress of the victim by putting themselves at risk of receiving aggression from the aggressor (Fraser et al. 2009). RBLs, as a different species, have no apparent relation with the aggressor, nor any long-term association with the macaque group. It is uncertain if post-conflict consolation happens between different primate species, yet genera (Kazem & Aureli 2005), but this might explain our observation. As the LTM groomers made their way to the higher strata and away from LTMs that competed for the provisioned food on ground, the LTM groomers might have found an opportunity to relieve stress by grooming the large female RBL who might simultaneously have deterred some other low-ranking macaques due to her body size (Schino & Aureli 2008a). In addition, there was no apparent competition between RBLs and LTMs over the provisioned anthropogenic food in this context. However, the possibility of LTMs solely seeking food, i.e. ectoparasite on langurs’ skin, from grooming, and RBLs’ desire for grooming for hygiene purposes, cannot be ruled out (Johnson et al. 2010).

The different duration of these interspecies grooming bouts during two independent observations was probably due to different extrinsic factors from the environment, and the intrinsic motivation of the primate individuals. The desire to be groomed usually follows a cyclical pattern of motivation and demotivation (Russell & Phelps 2013). Grooming is likely a crucial instigator of pleasure in primates, and the length of grooming time is the most available and applicable variable to reflect the quantity of the pleasurable effect (Russell & Phelps 2013). The female RBL recipient from the first observation was initially in a resting state before two young LTMs approached her and attempted to groom her. The relatively long grooming bout and her relaxed behaviour during the bout implied that she was not only tolerating but enjoying it. However, in the second observation, RBLs were already engaged in feeding while the young female LTM sought grooming opportunities, which were rejected or actively terminated by several individuals. It is unclear why the adult female RBLs tolerated the grooming in the first observation but not the second. A study on inter primates species interactions near Sungai Bernam, Malaysia, which included one of the closely related Presbytis langurs (older synonymization P. melalophos in the text; updated to either P. robinsoni or P. s. siamensis in the region) have concluded that intergenera interactions are typically non-competitive, and the association occurs with indication of selective preferences rather than by chance (Bernstein 1967).

In our study, both interspecies grooming events happened around dusk time, but there was a main difference in the RBL activities during both events. In the first observation, LTMs followed RBLs from the developed area back to the forest by crossing a road, and afterwards the two groups separated to reach their respective sleeping trees without further interaction. In the second observation, the female RBL was observed feeding and actively terminated the grooming session without aggression toward the groomer, ending the session as the RBLs headed to their sleeping trees. Judging from the several short grooming bouts in the second observation, RBLs likely prioritized feeding shortly before reaching their sleeping sites rather than losing foraging opportunities from being groomed by the macaques. Being groomed can also be painful as it involves pinching and pulling substances off the skin’s surface (Dunbar 2010). It was not obvious if the RBLs were uncomfortable or experiencing pain, especially when the groomer directly bit ectoparasites off the langurs’ skin.

Indeed, time spent on grooming is a significant part of primates’ activity budgets, but not much information related to grooming is published for Presbytis langurs. A study on Hanuman Langurs showed that recipients determine grooming spots on the body during allogrooming, directing the groomer also toward inaccessible body parts (Borries 1992). From our observation, RBLs autogroom themselves mostly on limb parts while resting, but no allogrooming between adult female RBLs or between mother-infant pairs have been observed in this study so far.  However, our study period is still considered short and the study troop is not fully habituated.

LTMs that have been regularly provisioned by humans may alter their natural behaviour exhibiting more flexibility and are likely more willing to engage in interspecies interactions (Sugiyama 2015). It is not uncommon to see interspecies interactions and grooming in captive environments, especially in zoos and sanctuaries (Silva 2017). In general, LTMs that regularly receive provisioning reduce their foraging distance, resulting in smaller home ranges, as concentrated food resources can be obtained nearby (Sha & Hanya 2013). This may increase the likelihood of interactions with humans and domestic animals in these anthropogenically modified environments (Bicca-Marques 2017). However, it also raises concerns about interspecies disease transmission as a spillover effect of the human-macaque interface (Gillespie et al. 2008; Rushmore et al. 2017; Balasubramaniam et al. 2020a,b). When macaques engage in interactions and close proximity with threatened primates, such as RBLs who are Critically Endangered, the risk of transmitting zoonotic diseases adds to the factors that already threaten this rare species (Ang et al. 2012, 2020).

In future studies, the frequency and ecological significance of interspecies grooming should be examined more thoroughly. The authors believe that this behaviour is more common in the wild than reflected by almost non-existent reports in literature. The role of anthropogenic factors, such as food provisioning and potential edge effects from habitat degradation should be investigated for their impacts on behavioural flexibility, which may facilitate interspecies interactions in disturbed sites. Cross-species transmission of diseases and parasites as a potential consequence of interspecies grooming should also be studied to reveal vulnerability of different primate species to potential pathogens.

 

 

Table 1. Interspecies grooming observation of Long-tailed Macaques (LTM) grooming Raffles’ Banded Langurs (RBL) and possible reason (SR—stress relieve with oxytocin hormone | Co—post conflict consolation | ER—ectoparasite removal | Fo—foraging for ectoparasite by actors | na—non-applicable).

Date

Raffles’ Banded Langur recipients

Long-tailed Macaque actors

Grooming duration

Possible reason or benefit from grooming

02.x.2020

Adult female 1

Subadult male 1 & 2

8 minutes

LTM: SR, Co, Fo

RBL: ER

08.x.2020

Juvenile female 1

Juvenile female 1

1 minutes 10 seconds

LTM: Fo

RBL: ER

08.x.2020

Juvenile female 2

Juvenile female 1

50 seconds

LTM: Fo

RBL: ER

08.x.2020

Adult female (Unidentified)

Juvenile female 1

1 second

LTM: Fo

RBL: na

08.x.2020

Adult female (Unidentified)

Juvenile female 1

28 seconds

LTM: Fo

RBL: ER

08.x.2020

Adult female (Unidentified)

Juvenile female 1

1 minutes 10 seconds

LTM: Fo

RBL: ER

 

 

For Images - - click here

 

 

References

 

Ang, A., A. Srivasthan, B.M. Md-Zain, M.R.B. Ismail & R. Meier (2012). Low genetic variability in the recovering urban banded leaf monkey population of Singapore. Raffles Bulletin of Zoology 60(2): 589–594.

Ang, A., V. D’Rozario, S.L. Jayasri, C.M. Lees, T.J. Li & S. Luz (2016). Species Action Plan for the Conservation of Raffles’ Banded Langur (Presbytis femoralis femoralis) in Malaysia and Singapore. IUCN SSC Conservation Breeding Specialist Group, Apple Valley, MN, USA.

Ang, A., D.I. Roesma, V. Nijman, R. Meier, A. Srivathsan & Rizaldi (2020). Faecal DNA to the rescue: Shotgun sequencing of non-invasive samples reveals two subspecies of Southeast Asian primates to be Critically Endangered species. Scientific Report 10: 9396.  https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020-66007-8

Aureli, F. (1992). Post-conflict behaviour among wild Long-tailed Macaques (Macaca fascicularis). Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 31: 329–337.

Baker, R.P. & D.G. Preston (1973). The effects of interspecies infant interaction upon social behavior of Macaca irus and Erythrocebus patas. Primates 14(4): 383–392. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01731359   

Balasubramaniam, K.N., C. Sueur, M.A. Huffman & A.J.J. MacIntosh (2020a). Primate Infectious Disease Ecology: Insight and Future Directions at the Human-Macaque Interface, pp. 249–284. In: Li, J.H., L. Sun. & P.P. Kappeler (eds.). The Behavioral Ecology of the Tibetan Macaque. Springer, Switzerland, 302pp.

Balasubramaniam, K.N., P.R. Marty, S. Samartino, A. Sobrino, G. Taniya, M. Ismail, R. Saha, B.A. Beisner, S.S.K. Kaburu, E. Bliss-Moreau, M.E. Arlet, N. Ruppert, A. Ismail, S.A.M. Sah, L. Mohan, S.K. Rattan, U. Kodandaramaiah & B. McCowan (2020b). Impact of individual demographic and social factors on human-wildlife interactions: a comparative study of three macaque species. Scientific Reports 10: 21991. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020-78881-3  

Barrett, L. & S.P. Henzi (2001). The utility of grooming in baboon troops, pp. 119–145. In: Noë, R., J.A.R.A.M. van Hooff & P. Hammerstein (eds.). Economics in nature: social dilemmas, mate choice and biological markets. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, 276 pp.

Bernstein, I.S. (1967). Intertaxa Interactions in a Malayan Primate Community. Folia Primatol. 7: 198–207. https://doi.org/10.1159/000155119  

Bicca-Marques, J.C. (2017). Urbanization (and primate conservation). pp. 1–5. In: Fuentes, A., M. Bezanson, C.J. Campbell, A.F. Di Fiore, S. Elton., A. Estrada & J. Yamagiwa (eds.). The International Encyclopedia of Primatology – 3 Volumes. Wiley-Blackwell, 1608 pp. https://doi.org/10.1002/9781119179313.wbprim0153  

Borries, C. (1992). Grooming site preferences in female Langurs (Presbytis entellus). International Journal of Primatology 13(1): 19–32. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02547725

Call, J., F. Aureli & F.B.M. de Waal (2002). Postconflict third-party affiliation in stump-tailed macaques. Animal Behaviour 63(2): 209–216. https://doi.org/10.1006/anbe.2001.1908  

Chua, L.S.L., M. Suhaida, M. Hamidah & L.G. Saw (2010). Malaysia Plant Red List: Peninsular Malaysian Dipterocarpaceae. Forest Research Institute Malaysia, Kepong, Malaysia, 210 pp.

Cords, M. (1992). Post-conflict reunions and reconciliation in long-tailed macaque. Animal Behaviour 44(1): 57–61. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0003-3472(05)80754-7  

Dunbar, R.I.M. (1991). Functional Significance of Social Grooming in Primates. Folia Primatologica 57: 121–131. https://doi.org/10.1159/000156574  

Dunbar, R.I.M. (2010). The social role of touch in humans and primates: behavioural function and neurobiological mechanisms. Neurosicence & Biobehavioral Reviews 34: 260–268. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2008.07.001  

Dunbar, R.I.M. (2011). Grooming, Gossip and the Evolution of Language. Clays Ltd, St Ives Plc, England, 230 pp.

Dunbar, R.I.M. & J. Lehman (2013). Grooming and social cohension in primates: a comment on Grueter et al. Evolution and Human Behaviour 34: 453–455. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2013.08.003  

Fraser, O.N., S.E. Koski, R.M. Witting & F. Aureli (2009). Why are bystanders friendly to recipients of aggression? Communicative & Integrative Biology 2(3): 285–291. https://doi.org/10.4161/cib.2.3.8718  

Gathua, M. (2000). Social interactions between two sympatric forest guenons, Cercopithecus ascanius and Cercopithecus mitis¸ in Kenya. Folia Primatologica 71: 353–355. https://doi.org/10.1159/000021760  

Gilhooly, L.J. & I.C. Colquhoun (2018). Variations in aggressive encounters between tourists and a hybrid macaque group (Macaca fascicularis x M. nemestrina) in Sabah, Malaysia. American Journal of Physical Anthropology 165: 98–99, Conference Abstract.

Gillespie, T.R., C.L. Nunn & F.H. Leendertz (2008). Integrative approaches to the study of primate infectious disease: implications for biodiversity conservation and global health. American Journal of Physical Anthropology 137(S47): 53–69. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajpa.20949  

Goosen, C. (1981). On the function of allogrooming in Old-World Monkey, pp. 110–120. In: Chiarelli, A.B. & R.S. Corruccini (eds.). Primate Behavior and Sociobiology. Proceedings in Life Sciences. Springer, Berlin, Heidelberg, 140 pp.

Grueter, C.C., A. Bissonette., K. Isler. & C.P. van Schaik (2013). Grooming and group cohesion in primates: implications for the evolution of language. Evolution and Human Behaviour 34: 61–68. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2012.09.004

Gumert, M.D. (2007). Grooming and infant handling interchange in Macaca fascicularis; The relationship between infant supply and grooming payment. International Journal of Primatology 28: 1059–1074. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10764-007-9202-0

Johnson, P.T.J., A. Dobson., K.D. Lafferty, D.J. Marcogliese, J. Memmott, S.A. Orlofske, R. Poulin. & D.W. Thieltges (2010). When parasites become prey: ecological and epidemiological significance of eating parasites. Trends in Ecology and Evolution 25(6): 362–371. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tree.2010.01.005

Kazem, A.J.N. & F. Aureli (2005). Redirection of aggression: multiparty signalling within a network, pp. 191–218. In: McGregor, P.J. (ed.). Animal Communication Networks. Cambridge University Press, 444pp.

Lazaro-Perea, C., M.D.F. Arruda & C.R. Snowdon (2004). Grooming as a reward? Social function of grooming between females in cooperatively breeding marmosets. Animal Behaviour 67: 627–636. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2003.06.004

Lehman, J., A.H. Korsthens & R.I.M. Dunbar (2007). Group size, grooming and social cohesion in primates. Animal Behaviour 74: 1617–629. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2006.10.025

Maple, T. & B. Westlund (1975). The integration of social interactions between cebus and spider monkeys in captivity. Applied Animal Ethology 1(3): 305–308. https://doi.org/10.1016/0304-3762(75)90024-3

McFarland R. & B. Majolo (2011). Grooming coercion and the post-conflict trading of social services in Wild Barbary Macaques. PLOS ONE 6(10): e26893. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0026893  

Matsuda, I., K. Fukaya, C. Pasquaretta & C. Sueur (2015). Factors Influencing Grooming Social Networks: Insights from Comparisons of Colobines with Different Dispersal Patterns, pp. 231–254. In: Furuichi, T., J. Yamagiwa & F. Aureli (eds.). Dispersing Primate Females: Life History and Social Strategies in Male-Philopatric Species. Springer Japan, 299pp.

Nerlekar, A. (2012). An instance of inter species interaction between Hanuman Langur (Semnopithecus entellus) and Rhesus Macaque (Macaca mulatta). Zoo’s Print XXII(8): 27–28.

Rushmore, J., D. Bisanzio & T.R. Gillespie (2017). Making new connections: insights from primate-parasite networks. Trends in Parasitology 33(7): 547–560.

Russell, Y.I. & S. Phelps (2013). How do you measure pleasure? A discussion about intrinsic costs and benefits in primate allogrooming. Biology & Philosophy 28: 1005–1020. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10539-013-9372-4

Schino, G. (2001). Grooming, competition and social rank among female primate: a meta-analysis. Animal Behaviour 62: 265–271. https://doi.org/10.1006/anbe.2001.1750

Schino, G. & F. Aureli (2008a). Trade-offs in primate grooming reciprocation: testing behavioural flexibility and correlated evolution. Biological Journal of the Linnean Society 95: 439–446. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1095-8312.2008.01067.x

Schino, G. & F. Aureli (2008b). Grooming reciprocation among female primates: a meta-analysis. Biology Letters 4: 9–11. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsbl.2007.0506  

Schino, G. & F. Aureli (2010). The relative roles of kinship and reciprocity in explaining primate altruism. Ecology Letters 13: 45–50. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1461-0248.2009.01396.x

Seyfarth, R.M. (1977). A model of social grooming among adult female monkeys. Journal of Theoretical Biology 65: 671–698.

Sha, J.C.M. & G. Hanya (2013). Diet, activity, habitat use, and ranging of two neighboring groups of food-enhanced long-tailed macaques (Macaca fascicularis). American Journal of Primatology 75: 581–592. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajp.22137

Silva, N. (2017).  Interspecies animal “Friendships”. MSc Thesis, Department of Animal Environment and Health, Swedish University of Agricultural Sciences, 54pp.

Sugiyama, Y. (2015). Influence of provisioning on primate behavior and primate studies. Mammalia 75(3): 255–265. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajp.22137

Tiddi, B., F. Aureli & G. Schino (2012). Grooming up the hierarchy: the exchange of grooming and rank-related benefits in a New World primate. PLoS ONE 7(5): e36641. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0036641

Tsuji, Y., M. Shimoda-Ishiguro, N. Ohnishi & S. Takatsuki (2007). A friend in need is a friend indeed: feeding association between Japanese Macaques and Sika Deer. Acta Theriologica 52(4): 427–434. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03194240

Vasava, A.G. & S. Mahato (2013). Grooming of Sambar (Rusa unicolor) by Rhesus Macaque (Macaca mulatta) in Sariska Tiger Reserve, Rajasthan, India. Current Science 104(3): 296–298.

Veá, J.J., A.P. Pérez, M. Baldellou & V. Alea (1999). Cost-benefit analysis of allogrooming behaviour in Cercocebus Torquatus lunulatus. Behaviour 136(2): 243-–256. https://brill.com/view/journals/beh/136/2/article-p243_6.xml

Wooddell, L.J., S.S.K. Kaburu, K.L. Rosenberg, J.S. Meyer, S.J. Suomi, A.M. Dettmer (2016). Matrilineal behavioral and physiological changes following the removal of a non-alpha matriarch in Rhesus Macaques (Macaca mulatta). PLoS ONE 11(11): e0167739. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0167739