Journal of Threatened Taxa | www.threatenedtaxa.org | 26 September 2019 | 11(12): 14471–14483

 

 

Ornithophony in the soundscape of Anaikatty Hills, Coimbatore, Tamil Nadu, India

 

Chandrasekaran Divyapriya 1  & Padmanabhan Pramod 2

 

1,2 Nature Education Division, Sálim Ali Centre for Ornithology & Natural History (SACON), Anaikatty (P.O.), Coimbatore, Tamil Nadu 641108, India.

1 Bharathiar University, Marudhamalai Road, Coimbatore, Tamil Nadu 641046, India.

1 cdp08india@gmail.com (corresponding author), 2 neosacon@gmail.com

 

 

 

Abstract: An attempt has been made to understand the extent of ornithophony (vocalization of birds) in the soundscape of Anaikatty Hills.  The study was limited to 13 hours of daylight from dawn to dusk (06.00–19.00 h) between January 2015 and October 2016.  Six replicates of 5-minute bird call recordings were collected from each hour window in 24 recording spots of the study area.  Each 5-minute recording was divided into 150 ‘2-sec’ observation units for the detailed analysis of the soundscape. A total of 78 recordings amounting to 390 minutes of acoustic data allowed a preliminary analysis of the ornithophony of the area.  A total of 62 bird species were heard vocalizing during the study period and contributed 8,629 units.  A total of 73.75% acoustic space was occupied by birds, among which the eight dominant species alone contributed to 63.65% of ornithophony.  The remaining 26% of acoustic space was occupied by other biophonies (12.60%), geophony (5.57%), indistinct sounds (7.66%), and anthropogenic noise (0.41%).  Passerines dominated the vocalizations with 7,269 (84.24%) and non-passerines with 1,360 (15.76%) units.  Birds vocalized in all 13 observation windows, with a peak in the first three hours of the day (06.00–09.00 h).  Vocalizations of non-passerines were prominent in the dusk hours (18.00–19.00 h).

 

Keywords: Acoustic community, bird acoustics, bird vocalization, diurnal singing, ornithophony, soundscape analysis.

 

 

 

doi: https://doi.org/10.11609/jott.4948.11.12.14471-14483   

 

Editor: V.V. Robin, Department of Biology, IISER Tirupati, India. Date of publication: 26 September 2019 (online & print)

 

Manuscript details: #4948 | Received 13 March 2019 | Final received 02 August 2019 | Finally accepted 21 August 2019

 

Citation: Divyapriya, C. & P. Pramod (2019). Ornithophony in the soundscape of Anaikatty Hills, Coimbatore, Tamil Nadu, India. Journal of Threatened Taxa 11(12): 14471–14483. https://doi.org/10.11609/jott.4948.11.12.14471-14483

 

Copyright: © Divyapriya & Pramod 2019. Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License.  JoTT allows unrestricted use, reproduction, and distribution of this article in any medium by adequate credit to the author(s) and the source of publication.

 

Funding: The study was self-funded by CD.

 

Competing interests: The authors declare no competing interests.

 

Author details and contribution: C. Divyapriya is a PhD scholar in Nature Education Division, Sálim Ali Centre for Ornithology & Natural History (SACON). She collected, curated, analyzed and interpreted the audio recordings of birds and major contributor in writing the manuscript.  The work is part of her doctoral thesis.  Dr. P. Pramod is a Principal Scientist, Sálim Ali Centre for Ornithology & Natural History (SACON) and Head of the Nature Education division. He conceptualized, supervised the study, reviewed the analysis and edited the draft.  All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

 

Acknowledgements: Authors thank The Cornell Lab of Ornithology for providing RAVEN Pro 1.4 version with 100% concession. Authors thank director, Sálim Ali Centre for Ornithology and Natural History (SACON), Coimbatore for providing facilities and encouragement.

 

 

 

INTRODUCTION

 

The biological sound produced by vocalizing animals (e.g., birds and stridulating insects (biophony)), non-biological sounds such as wind, rain, running stream (geophony) in a forest or any natural habitat (Hildebrand 2009) constitutes the soundscape of that area (Pijanowski et al. 2011; Gage & Axel 2014).  The man-made sounds produced from automobile, machinery (technophony or anthrophony) that dominate in urban settings are rarely detected in forest habitats (Krause 1987; Pijanowski et al. 2011; Gage & Axel 2014).  Vocalization of birds (ornithophony) of a terrestrial habitat varies due to the variations in the dominant vocalizers, number of species involved in vocal activity and the time specificity of the birds.  It is well known that many species of birds are more vocally active during dawn and dusk hours as they are active in search of food and / or attracting a female partner (Slabbekoorn 2004; Brumm, 2006; Catchpole & Slater 2008; Ey & Fischer 2009).  Leaving aside the functionality, ornithophony is observed as one of the dominant aspects of the soundscape of any natural ecosystem, especially in forests.

The vocal communication of the birds was well studied, experimented and the results give insights about the characteristics of avian vocal signals (Aylor 1971; Morton 1975; Wiley & Richards 1978; Brenowitz 1982).  The environmental factors such as humidity, temperature, atmospheric turbulence, or vegetation cover influence the signal transfer through masking, absorption, attenuation, reverberation or signal scattering effect (Wiley & Richards 1978).  Birds prefer a suitable environmental condition for the effective long-distant signal transfer (Morton 1975; Kroodsma 1977; Brenowitz 1982). As the vocal communication consumes significant energy and time (Prestwich 1994; Oberweger & Goller 2001), animals adapt their vocal signals spectrally, by altering their syllable structure and usage; or temporally, by opting for a better daytime hour for signal transfer (Ficken et al. 1974; Nelson & Marler 1990; Boncoraglio & Saino 2007; Planque & Slabbekoorn 2008; Ey & Fischer 2009; Velásquez et al. 2018).  Birds reduce the interference and masking effect of other animal signals such as insects (Stanley et al. 2016), and abiotic noise like wind and water (Klump 1996).  Hence, birds have vocal partitioning or an  ‘acoustic niche’ (Brumm 2006; Planque & Slabbekoorn 2008; Luther 2009; Hart et al. 2015).  As dawn and dusk hours have a favourable environmental conditions (Morton 1975; Slagsvold 1996; Hutchinson 2002) and enhance long-distant signal transfer (Henwood & Fabrick 1979; Dabelsteen & Mathevon 2002; Brown & Handford 2003), birds probably prefer those hours for consistent signal transfer.

The interaction of biological and non-biological sounds provides the overall framework of the acoustic ecology of a landscape (Pijanowski et al. 2011).  Spectral frequency (Hz) analysis is a valid method for interpreting the terrestrial soundscape (Irwin 1990; Nowicki & Nelson 1990; Cardoso 2010; Cardoso & Atwell 2011).  Overlapping of sound frequencies of geophony (such as wind, rain) or technophony (automobiles) may mask the biophony signals (Qi et al. 2008; Mullet 2017).  Most of the technophony and a few biophonic sounds (birds) occur in lower frequency range 1–2 kHz.  Passerines species’ frequency ranges between 3 and 6 kHz, whereas insects occupy a higher range, > 6kHz, and all the geophony are of low frequency ranging from 1–11 kHz (Napoletano 2004; Qi et al. 2008; Joo et al. 2011; Kasten et al. 2012; Gage & Axel 2014).

Biophony of the soundscape can be comprehended by examining the temporal framework across the daytime from dawn to dusk (Joo 2008; Joo et al. 2011).  It also provides valuable insights on species diversity (Napoletano 2004; Sueur et al. 2008) and ecosystem (Qi et al. 2008).  This study is a first step to understand the biophony in the soundscape of Anaikatty Hills through a community acoustics’ approach on the ornithophony across daylight hours.

 

 

METHODS

 

Study area

The study area is Anaikatty Hills (11.090–11.097 0N & 76.778–76.792 0E; Fig. 1), in Coimbatore District, Tamil Nadu, India, is a part of the Nilgiri Biosphere Reserve (NBR), approximately 500 to 600 m, lies on the leeward side of the Western Ghats. It receives an annual rainfall of about 700mm, which is mainly contributed by the north-east monsoon.  The temperature varies from 17˚ C to 36˚ C (Mukherjee & Bhupathy 2007).  It is a secondary forest area surrounded by dry deciduous forests rich in biodiversity and forms a part of the Western Ghats, which is one among the 35 biodiversity hotspots of the world (Noss et al. 2015).  The study site is dominated by trees such as Ceylon Tea Cassine glauca, Woolly-leaved Fire-brand Teak Premna tomentosa, Umbrella Thorn Acacia planifrons, Neem Azadirachta indica, Ceylon Boxwood Psydrax dicoccos, Krishna Siris Albizia amara, Bidi Leaf Tree Bauhinia racemosa, Algaroba Prosopis juliflora, and shrubs such as Orangeberry Glycosmis mauritiana, Clausena dentata, Cat Thorn Scutia myrtina, Siam Weed Chromolaena odorata, and Lantana Lantana camara (Balasubramanian et al. 2017).  A total of 145 bird species, from 48 families with 52% of passerine species has been reported from the study site (Ali et al. 2013).

 

Field methods

The acoustic signals were recorded from 24 different recording spots (Fig. 1) of the landscape to capture the soundscape from the maximum microhabitats from January 2015 to October 2016.  The study area is a scrub jungle with dry deciduous forest patches (Ali et al. 2013).  Acoustic data was recorded using Sony PCM-M10 portable linear PCM handheld audio recorder (2009), with an Audio-Technica ATR-6550 condenser shotgun microphone in .WAV format with 44.1kHz sampling frequency and 24-bit accuracy rate.  The diel pattern of acoustic behavior of birds was observed and calls were recorded from 06.00h to 19.00h spanning 13 hours of a day.  The daylight period is segmented into 13 one-hour slots (from henceforth mentioned as ‘observation window’).  Six replicates of 5-minute bird call recordings were collected from each window, of which each 5-minute call recording is considered as ‘a sampling unit’.  The first author held the microphone for one minute in each direction to capture the soundscape.  The sampling effort is six replications of 13h, makes 78 recordings. The average sampling effort per location was 3.0.  The sampling effort is presented in Table 1.  The recording date, time and location were noted during the recording period.  Recordings were not collected during rainy days.  The sunrise and sunset time was 06.00–06.48 h and 17.57–18:51 h, respectively.  The sunrise and sunset data were obtained from the official website of Indian Meteorological Department, Government of India.

 

Data analysis

Each 5-min recording was analysed by dividing it into 150 ‘2-sec’ parts (henceforth mentioned as ‘observation unit(s)’).  The first author manually investigated each 2-sec unit for capturing the dominant vocalizing bird species.  It was a challenging and time-consuming task, however, it helped to understand the soundscape in a much finer resolution.  About 90% of the species were identified and the remaining were documented as unidentified species.  One second would be too short, whereas 3-sec part would miss out the short vocal signals, hence, 2-sec unit analysis was preferred.  The term ‘vocal unit’ is used to refer to any biophony (animal vocalizations) present in it.  The calls/audio signals of (i) individual birds, (ii) unidentified birds, (iii) birds which were identified to their genus category, (iv) gap during the absence of any vocal signal of bird, (v) wind, (vi) vehicle noise, (vii) sound of other animals like Spotted Deer, Indian Palm Squirrel, goat, and (viii) other indistinct sounds were also noted in each observation unit.  The loud and vocally dominant species in each observation unit was visually classified and considered for further analysis.  The vocalizations identified to group level were also considered as separate taxa for broad level classifications, however, they are not included as separate species while accounting for the total number of species vocalized.

The 13 daytime hours were classified into morning (06.00–09.00 h), mid-day (09.00–12.00 h), afternoon (12.00–16.00 h), and evening (16.00–19.00 h) hours.  To study the variation on the number of bird species and vocal units across 13 observation windows, ANOVA test (Fisher 1925) with random effect was performed.  Kruskal-Wallis test (Kruskal & Wallis 1952) was performed to show statistical proof for significant variation between morning and evening hours against mid-day and afternoon hours.  All the statistical tests were performed using SPSS v.16.0 (SPSS Inc. 2007).  The sound recordings were analyzed for spectrogram views with the aid of sound analysis software Raven Pro 1.4 (Bioacoustics Research Program 2011) and audio signals were edited using Audacity 2.0.6. software.  The spectrogram settings in Raven Pro 1.4 (2011) were as follows: Hann 512, 3dB filter Bandwidth 124Hz, 50% overlap, grid spacing 86.1Hz.  The frequency values of bird vocalizations were measured by visual inspection method (Irwin 1990; Nowicki & Nelson 1990; Baker & Boylan 1995; Cardoso & Atwell 2011; Singh & Price 2015).

 

RESULTS

 

Soundscape analysis

The acoustic data collected from the field had 78 recordings with a total duration of 390 minutes sampled from multiple locations (24) of the same landscape evenly spread along the 13 different observation windows.  This gives 900 observation units per window adding to 11,700 units in total.  Visual classification of these observation units yielded a total of 62 bird species’ calls (Tables 2, 3).  The checklist of species was prepared following Praveen et al. (2019).  Passerines dominated all through the 13 day-hours and non-passerines were more vocalizing during 18.00h to 19.00h.  Especially, the first three hours had 19, 22, and 20 passerine species (Fig. 2). Thirty-nine passerine species (62.90%) and 23 (37.09%) non-passerine species (Tables 2, 3) were recorded as the vocalizers of the Anaikatty soundscape.  Among the total 11,700 observation units, birds occupied 8,629 (74%); of these, passerines occupied 7,269 (84.24%), and non-passerines only 1,360 (15.76%) vocal units (Fig. 3). Of the remaining 26% of the sample, 12.60% was contributed by biophony of other creature such as insects and 5.57% by geophony (wind, indistinct noise). Undetectable or indistinct sounds were 7.66%, and the remaining negligible 0.41% by anthropogenic noise.  ANOVA (Fisher 1925) showed that the bird species and vocal units significantly varied across the 13 observation windows, i.e., F12,65 = 4.220, p < 0.01 and F12,65 = 2.251, p = 0.019, respectively.  ANOVA (Fisher 1925) showed that the vocalization number of bird species were significantly varied across 13 hours (random effect in ANOVA).

 

Bird vocalizations across diurnal hours

The number of species recorded vocalizing was high in the initial three hours of the day (Fig. 2).  In the first hour of observation, i.e., 06.00–07.00 h, 95% of the time was occupied by bird calls (858 out of 900 observation units), 10.00–11.00 h window received the next maxima with 763 bird vocal units, and in the evening just before the sunset, i.e., 17.00–18.00 h had the next peak with 647 vocal units. (Fig. 3, 4).

The Kruskal-Wallis test showed no significant difference across the bird species between mid-day–afternoon hours against morning–evening hours, χ2 = 3.47, df = 1, p = 0.063 (N = 13).  There was no significant variation in vocal units among the tested groups χ2 = 0.73, df = 1, p = 0.39 (N = 13).  In any one-hour observational window, a minimum of 16 species was recorded to be vocally active.

Non-passerines were higher at 06.00–07.00 h and declined as the day progressed.  There was a peak in their vocalizations during 18.00–19.00 h (Fig. 2).  It is to be noted that non-passerine vocal contribution increased from 15.00h onwards (Fig. 3).  Among the 13 hours, Indian Pitta was more vocal during 18.00–19.00 h.  The 15 species that contributed to dusk calls were either producers of low-frequency calls or harmonics.  Totally, 10 species (Yellow-billed Babbler, Jungle Crow, Common Tailorbird, Indian Peafowl, Indian Robin, White-browed Bulbul, Spotted Dove, Red-vented Bulbul, Grey Jungle fowl, and Common Hawk Cuckoo) were observed to be vocalizing both in dawn and dusk time.  The low and high frequency values of the 62 species are given in Tables 2 and 3.

 

Dominance in vocalization

Eight species dominated the ornithophony with 63.65% of vocal units’ contribution (Fig. 5 and their statistical analysis is provided in Table 4).  Of these, Common Tailorbird, Red-vented Bulbul, Yellow-billed Babbler, Indian Robin, and White-browed Bulbul had vocalized in all 13-hour observation windows (Fig. 5), whereas Purple-rumped Sunbird, Grey-breasted Prinia, and Common Iora were absent in the 18.00–19.00 h window.  Common Tailorbird dominated the soundscape of the study area with 1,619 vocal units (Fig. 5), i.e., 18.76% vocal signal contribution and was present in 74 out of 78 recordings.  White-browed Bulbul’s vocal signals were present in 66 recordings, occupied just 3.97% of total ornithophony (Table 4).  Indian Paradise-flycatcher was found only in a 5-min recording.  They produce several quick high-pitched notes and hence, occupy several observation units (40) in a single utterance.  The Common Rose-finch, Blue-bearded Bee-eater, Rose-ringed Parakeet, Indian Golden Oriole, Ashy Drongo, Plum-headed Parakeet, Tawny-bellied Babbler, Greater Racket-tailed Drongo, and Barn Swallow were observed in only one of the recordings.

Fifteen non-passerines were recorded vocalizing during the dawn hour (06.00–07.00 h), after that non-passerine composition declined in the subsequent hours (Fig. 2).  It is to be noted that non-passerines vocal contribution slightly increased from 15.00h onwards (Fig. 3).  Indian Peafowl, Grey Francolin, Grey Junglefowl, Red-wattled Lapwing, Jerdon’s Nightjar, and Common Hawk Cuckoo were the dominant non-passerines during the 18.00–19.00 h window and were at low ebb or almost nil during other hours.  Indian Peafowl was the only non-passerine to be vocally active in all 13 observation windows, the Grey Francolins were present in seven out of 13 observation windows, and the Grey Junglefowl calls were recorded in six observation windows.  Indian Pitta being a winter visitor and lower song rate species had fewer vocal units in the present study.  Figure 6 shows the number of bird species’ spread in each observation window.  The 06.00–08.00 h window had more bird species, whereas, 18.00–19.00 h had the least.  Figure 7 depicts the vocal units’ data spread.  Vocal units at 09.00–10.00 h, 12.00–13.00 h, and 18.00–19.00 h were relatively more variable than other observation hours.

 

 

DISCUSSION

 

Soundscape analysis

The study area, a scrub jungle in a dry deciduous landscape, had more of sound than silence in day hours.  The sounds of birds dominated 74% of the time in the study area, especially in the initial three hours.  We have recorded other biophony and indistinct, undetectable sound sources from the study area.  The indistinct sounds in the study area could be relatively short-bursts of wind or sound produced by any other vocalizing animal.  Earlier studies say that the forest environment has lesser decibel (Aylor 1971; Marten & Marler 1977; Marten et al. 1977) as background sound than in urban areas (Brumm & Slabbekoorn 2005; Brumm 2006).  The terrestrial habitats are prone to low-frequency noise caused by air turbulence, rain, running water (Brumm & Slabbekoorn 2005) and other biotic noises (Slabbekoorn 2004).  The omnipresent cicadas and their concert produce a constant spectrum of background noise (Slabbekoorn 2004).  Therein, the biophony generally ranges between 2kHz and 11kHz (Napoletano 2004; Qi et al. 2008; Joo et al. 2011; Kasten et al. 2012; Gage & Axel 2014).  Mullet et al. (2016) clarify that the high-frequency vocalizing passerines can be effectively distinguished from low-frequency producers through a spectrogram analysis.  To avoid the biological or non-biological sound frequency overlap, birds utilize different acoustic niches to broadcast the information (Krause 1987; Qi et al. 2008; Luther 2009).

This acoustic diversity study assessed the ornithophony distribution across day hours.  Anaikatty soundscape has 86.60% of biophony. Gage & Axel’s (2014) soundscape power analysis study of Cheboygan County soundscape showed that the biological sounds attributed to 80% of total eco-acoustics. The frequency-dependent acoustic analysis corroborates that ornithophony occupies the 2–8 kHz of spectral bandwidth (Napoletano 2004; Qi et al. 2008; Gage & Axel 2014).  Thus, acoustic diversity study across the day hours will assess the ornithophony distribution and assess the soundscape framework of a habitat.

 

Bird vocalizations across diurnal hours

More number of species showed acoustic activity in dawn and dusk hours; however, the vocal units were not significantly different across 13 hours.  The soundscape of the study area had higher bird vocalizations in the early three hours (0600–09.00 h).  The temperature, wind, humidity is more advantageous with least atmospheric turbulence and less background noise during dawn, thus enhancing the signal transmission (Morton 1975; Kroodsma 1977; Krebs & Davies 1981; Slagsvold 1996; Hutchinson 2002; Luther 2009; Hart et al. 2015). Early hour bird vocalizations were observed in Arizona and in Kutai Nature Reserve, Borneo (Henwood & Fabrick 1979), deciduous forest in Denmark (Dabelsteen & Mathevon 2002), open grassland and closed forest habitat in Ontario (Brown & Handford 2003), and upland pasture at New York (Brenowitz 1982).  Moreover, the dawn (and dusk) chorus gives the advantage to use the energy reserve unused since the previous night (McNamara et al. 1987; Hutchinson 2002).  Dawn chorus also has reproductive benefits such as attracting a mate and deter other potent males to get access to the partner (Slagsvold 1996; Catchpole & Slater 2008), to defend territory and nest site from conspecific males (Slagsvold 1996).

Low frequency and/or harmonic producing birds’ vocalizations dominated the dusk hour (18.00–19.00 h; Tables 2,3).  Low frequency vocalizations of birds and amphibians dominated during the night at Cheboygan County, Michigan (Gage & Axel 2014). Harmonics increases the difficulty in locating the calling bird (Blindfolded birdwatching 2010), thus avoiding predatory attacks.  As the visual cues are undependable during the sunset hour (Kacelnik 1979), low frequency gives an advantage for long-distance signal propagation (Aylor 1971; Morton 1975; Marten & Marler 1977; Martenet al. 1977; Wiley & Richards 1982; Wiley 1991).  Song activity at dusk increases the pair-bonding behavior in American Robins (Slagsvold 1996), and in Blackbird (Cuthill & Macdonald 1990).   A peak in dawn and dusk vocal activity suggest that these hours are important for a male to guard the mate and nest site (Sturkie 1976; Mace 1986, 1987; Cuthill & Macdonald 1990).  Soundscape peaked at dawn chorus (06.00–07.00 h), then dropped shortly after sunrise, till evening and once again raised during dusk hours and reached second maxima at 20.00h in Cheboygan County, Michigan (Gage & Axel 2014).

 

Dominance in vocalization

The Common Tailorbird was the most dominant vocalizer of the landscape as their calls were louder and have a higher song rate, i.e., the number of call syllables produced in a minute.  All the eight dominant species  vocalize continuously.  The passerines are louder and are continuous vocalizers (Garamszegi & Møller 2004; Catchpole & Slater 2008; Cardoso 2010).  Seven of the dominant species are forage generalists and were vocally active all through the day yielding a higher vocal unit.  The early hours had uniform vocal units’ contribution per observation window.  Increased variability of vocal units during 09.00–10.00 h, 12.00–13.00 h, 14.00–15.00 h, and 18.00–19.00 h could be attributed to relatively variable number of vocalizers (Fig. 7).  This might also show the need of more sampling efforts.

The 16.00–17.00 h observation window had more non-passerines (11 species) yielding fewer vocal units, whereas, passerines were predominant in the study area with more vocal units.  More vocal units and complexity exhibits the versatility of passerine birds (Garamszegi & Møller 2004; Boncaraglio & Saino 2007; Catchpole & Slater 2008; Cardoso 2010), as they are louder (Calder 1990; Cardoso & Mota 2009; Cardoso 2010) and are continuous vocalizers (Hartley & Suthers 1989; Irwin 1990; Podos 1997; Forstmeier et al. 2002).  This makes passerines to occupy a larger portion of the soundscape of Anaikatty Hills in general.

Song rate analysis is beyond the scope of this present study, however, any trained ears could relatively understand the song rate of bird calls.  The study which aimed at understanding the vocal activity pattern of diurnal birds illustrates that the soundscape of Anaikatty is largely occupied by birds in those hours.

 

 

CONCLUSIONS

 

Birds occupy 73.75% of acoustic space in the soundscape of Anaikatty Hills and the remaining 26.25% includes the vocal activity of insects, other indistinct sounds or complete silence.  Thirty-nine passerine species (62.90%) and 23 non-passerine species (37.09%) vocalized in the sampled soundscape of the study area.  The eight dominant species constitutes 63.65% of ornithophony of the study area.  Out of the total sampled ornithophony, passerines occupied 84.35% and non-passerines 14.74% of the vocal units.  Birds vocalized in all 13 daylight hours, with a peak in the first three hours of the day (06.00–09.00 h).  Passerines dominated the soundscape in all hours except the dusk 18.00–19.00 h.

 

Limitation of the study

The sampling effort was done to answer the preliminary account of ornithophony of the soundscape of the region.  Though the researcher intentionally did not direct the microphone towards the vocalizing bird, the usage of shotgun microphone might have had an effect on the calling bird.  Though the researcher had sampled the 5-min by directing the microphone in all directions, the shotgun microphone was a limitation for the soundscape study compared to the omnidirectional microphone.

 

 

Table 1. Sampling effort of the study in Anaikatty Hills.

 

13 hrs/

24 loc

1

2

3

4

5

6

7

8

9

10

11

12

13

14

15

16

17

18

19

20

21

22

23

24

6–7 h

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

7-8 h

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

8–9 h

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

9–10 h

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

10–11 h

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

11–12 h

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

12–13 h

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

13–14 h

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

14–15 h

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

15–16 h

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

16–17 h

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

17–18 h

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

18–19 h

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

The sampling effort was distributed across 13 hours in 24 locations to capture the soundscape of the study area.

 

 

Table 2. List of passerine species of Anaikatty Hills recorded during the study. Birds with harmonics are marked with an asterisk (*). Sample size of the low and high frequencies are 10, except # - sample size 5; ^ - sample size 4.

 

 

Bird species /Family

Scientific name

Low-frequency values

(in Hz) (Mean ± S.D.)

High-frequency values

(in Hz) (Mean ± S.D.)

 

Pittidae

 

 

 

1

Indian Pitta

Pitta brachyura

1662.5 ± 289.5

4662.9 ±3353.1

 

Oriolidae

 

 

 

2

Black-hooded Oriole

Oriolus xanthornus

1465.97 ± 798.58

2229.97 ± 564.44

3

Eurasian Golden Oriole

 Oriolus oriolus

1099.7 ± 408.8

7825.8 ± 6266.1

 

Aegithinidae

 

 

 

4

Common Iora

Aegithina tiphia

1589.54 ± 301.49

3432.68 ± 682.08

 

Dicruridae

 

 

 

5

Ashy Drongo*

Dicrurus leucophaeus

1661.9 ± 329.3

10420.0 ± 3202.1

6

Greater Racket-tailed Drongo

Dicrurus paradiseus

1673.6 ± 118.9

2741.6 ± 53.9

 

Laniidae

 

 

 

7

Brown Shrike*

Lanius cristatus

2166.9 ± 504.1

10701.9 ±1479.1

 

Corvidae

 

 

 

8

Rufous Treepie*

Dendrocitta vagabunda

815.2 ± 272.5

18059.0 ± 1996.3

9

House Crow*

Corvus splendens

1205.1 ± 955.5

3136.6 ± 1317.2

10

Large-billed Crow*

Corvus macrorhynchos

1193.6 ± 690.6

2298.2 ± 658.7

 

Monarchidae

 

 

 

11

Indian Paradise-flycatcher*

Terpsiphone paradisi

1231.56 ± 262.78

13764.35 ± 1550.62

 

Dicaeidae

 

 

 

12

Thick-billed Flowerpecker

Dicaeum agile

2562.6 ± 602.4

14147.4 ± 592.3

13

Pale-billed Flowerpecker

Dicaeum erythrorhynchos

3721.5 ± 549.8

11403.5 ± 567.2

 

Nectariniidae

 

 

 

14

Purple-rumped Sunbird

Leptocoma zeylonica

3581.8 ± 461.5

6273.3 ± 1006.4

15

Purple Sunbird

Cinnyris asiaticus

4145.5 ± 1099.1

7016 ± 734.1

16

Loten's Sunbird

Cinnyris lotenius

4145.5 ± 662.3

6643.9 ±1530.6

 

Chloropseidae

 

 

 

17

Jerdon's Leafbird*

Chloropsis jerdoni

1844.6 ± 460.3

7736.8 ± 5421.0

 

Fringillidae

 

 

 

18

Common Rosefinch#

Carpodacus erythrinus

2060.1 ± 146.1

6003.3 ± 166.8

 

Paridae

 

 

 

19

Cinereous Tit

Parus cinereus

2835.5 ± 350.4

8553.6 ± 427.4

 

Cisticolidae

 

 

 

20

Grey-breasted Prinia

Prinia hodgsonii

3002.7 ± 329.6

7107.9 ± 325.6

21

Jungle Prinia

Prinia sylvatica

2705.6 ± 244.5

6545.5 ± 600.1

22

Ashy Prinia

Prinia socialis

2821.5 ± 530.2

6394.2 ± 611.4

23

Common Tailorbird

Orthotomus sutorius

2604.27 ± 1153.85

5840.91 ± 833.58

 

Acrocephalidae

 

 

 

24

Blyth's Reed Warbler

Acrocephalus dumetorum

2663.7 ± 505.34

7379.51 ± 335.14

 

Hirundinidae

 

 

 

25

Red-rumped Swallow*

Cecropis daurica

2719.4 ± 196.9

7807.4 ± 1334.1

26

Barn Swallow*

Hirundo rustica

2587.8 ± 597.3

8021.2 ± 2566.4

 

Pycnonotidae

 

 

 

27

Red-whiskered Bulbul

Pycnonotus jocosus

1703.8 ± 509.9

3667.3 ± 488.7

28

Red-vented Bulbul

Pycnonotus cafer

1562.8 ± 194.1

3062.5 ± 393.1

29

White-browed Bulbul

Pycnonotus luteolus

1256.8 ± 227.8

3707.7 ± 504.8

 

Phylloscopidae

 

 

 

30

Greenish Leaf Warbler

Phylloscopus trochiloides

3438.2 ± 716.6

7505.9 ± 1717.6

 

Timaliidae

 

 

 

31

Indian Scimitar Babbler*^

Pomatorhinus horsfieldii

622.7 ± 116.9

1300.2 ± 248.2

32

Tawny-bellied Babbler

Dumetia hyperythra

3475.0 ± 554.3

6443.7 ± 193.6

 

Leiothrichidae

 

 

 

33

Yellow-billed Babbler*

Turdoides affinis

3702.7 ± 518.8

9946.6 ± 2710.5

 

Sturnidae

 

 

 

34

Common Myna*

Acridotheres tristis

1399.8 ± 393.8

10244.5 ±3148.6

35

Jungle Myna*

Acridotheres fuscus

1368.7 ± 204.5

9803.4 ± 3469.0

 

Muscicapidae

 

 

 

36

Indian Robin

Saxicoloides fulicatus

5034.9 ± 1375.7

7261.5 ± 642.1

37

Oriental Magpie Robin*

Copsychus saularis

2399.4 ± 320.9

6770.0 ± 2349.3

38

Tickell's Blue flycatcher

Cyornis tickelliae

3095.0 ± 206.8

7318.3 ± 1788.8

39

Pied Bushchat

Saxicola caprata

2037.4 ± 349.7

5089.6 ± 849.5

 

 

 

Table 3. List of non-passerine species of Anaikatty Hills recorded during the study. Birds with harmonics are marked with an asterisk (*). The sample size for low and frequencies of the species are ten, except ^ - sample size is 8.

 

 

Bird species /Family

Scientific name

Low-frequency values

 (in Hz) (Mean ± S.D.)

High-frequency values

(in Hz) (Mean ± S.D.)

 

Phasianidae

 

 

 

1

Indian Peafowl*

Pavo cristatus

551.36 ± 84.9

10284.2 ± 891.5

2

Grey Francolin*

Francolinus pondicerianus

1908.2 ± 106.1

6700.1 ± 1873.2

3

Grey Junglefowl*

Gallus sonneratii

763.5 ± 647.6

8009.7 ± 4212.4

 

Columbidae

 

 

 

4

Spotted Dove

Streptopelia chinensis

569.0 ± 44.2

837.9 ± 39.6

5

Laughing Dove

Streptopelia senegalensis

640.8 ± 26.4

886.1 ± 22.9

 

Caprimulgidae

 

 

 

6

Jerdon's Nightjar

Caprimulgus atripennis

574.9 ± 41.2

1476.0 ± 30.8

 

Cuculidae

 

 

 

7

Greater Coucal

Centropus sinensis

398.0 ± 102.5

870.5 ± 233.4

8

Asian Koel*

Eudynamys scolopaceus

982.3 ± 75.49

10473.3 ± 4694.39

9

Common Hawk Cuckoo

Hierococcyx varius

1510.81 ± 357.50

2225.95 ± 280.65

 

Charadriidae

 

 

 

10

Red-wattled Lapwing*

Vanellus indicus

1490.9 ± 431.3

8282.1 ± 4678.9

 

Accipitridae

 

 

 

11

Crested Serpent Eagle*

Spilornis cheela

1806.7 ± 91.9

6317.6 + 1242.54

12

Shikra*

Accipiter badius

1472.9 ± 453.0

13709.4 ± 1980.1

 

Upupidae

 

 

 

13

Common Hoopoe

Upupa epops

795.0 ± 410.2

1621.1 ± 1052.1

 

Megalaimidae

 

 

 

14

White-cheeked Barbet

Psilopogon viridis

940.8 ± 61.7

1307.6 ± 40.2

15

Coppersmith Barbet

Psilopogon haemacephalus

633.8 ± 25.1

898.1 ± 25.4

 

Meropidae

 

 

 

16

Blue-bearded Bee-eater

Nyctyornis athertoni

586.17 ± 80.15

3740.23 ± 695.06

17

Green Bee-eater

Merops orientalis

2781.7 ± 219.5

4373.6 ± 241.5

18

Chestnut-headed Bee-eater

Merops leschenaulti

2538.88 ± 113.84

3590.01 ± 215.33

 

Alcedinidae

 

 

 

19

White-throated Kingfisher*

Halcyon smyrnensis

2436.2 ± 105.3

7272.7 ± 2739.7

 

Psittaculidae

 

 

 

20

Plum-headed Parakeet*^

Psittacula cyanocephala

1828.0 ± 468.1

6735.8 ± 1347.2

21

Malabar Parakeet*

Psittacula columboides

2571.6 ± 165.1

4199.9 ± 277.9

22

Rose-ringed Parakeet*

Psittacula krameri

2047.4 ± 798.9

8566.3 ± 1257.9

23

Vernal Hanging Parrot*

Loriculus vernalis

6261.7 ± 571.0

7948.1 ± 179.5

 

 

Table 4. Descriptive statistics of the eight most vocalizing passerines of the study area.

 

Bird sp.

Mean

Std. Dev.

Co-efficient of Variation (CV)

Min

Max

No. of presence among 78 recordings

No. of vocal units

Common Tailorbird

20.76

15.43

74.32

1.00

61.00

74

1619

Red-vented Bulbul

10.73

10.36

96.53

1.00

45.00

69

837

Common Iora

10.42

16.47

158.06

1.00

63.00

52

813

Yellow-billed Babbler

7.13

10.47

146.83

1.00

58.00

54

556

Purple-rumped Sunbird

7.09

11.61

163.72

1.00

68.00

52

553

Indian Robin

5.31

7.99

150.55

1.00

36.00

55

414

Grey-breasted Prinia

4.59

9.60

209.14

1.00

41.00

29

358

White-browed Bulbul

4.40

4.19

95.38

1.00

18.00

66

343

 

 

For figures – click here

 

 

REFERENCES

 

Ali, A.M.S., S.B. Shanthakumar, S.R. Kumar, R. Chandran, S.S. Marimuthu & P.R. Arun (2013). Birds of the Sálim Ali Centre for Ornithology and Natural History Campus, Anaikatty Hills, southern India. Journal of Threatened Taxa 5(17): 5288–5298. https://doi.org/10.11609/JoTT.3660.5288-98

Aylor, D. (1971). Noise reduction by vegetation and ground. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 51(1): 197–205. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.1912830

Balasubramanian, P., M.K. Sebastian, P.R. Arun, P. Pramod, R. Jayapal & H.N. Kumara (2017). Glimpses of SACON Campus Biodiversity. Sálim Ali Centre for Ornithology and Natural History, 81pp.

Baker, M.C. & J.T. Boylan (1995). A catalog of song syllables of Indigo and Lazuli Buntings. The Condor 97: 1028–1040. https://doi.org/10.2307/1369541

Bioacoustics Research Program (2011). Raven Pro: Interactive Sound Analysis Software (Version 1.4) [Computer software]. Ithaca, NY: The Cornell Lab of Ornithology. Available from http://www.birds.cornell.edu/raven.

Blindfolded birdwatching (2010). The effect of harmonics on localization of birds calls (May 30). Retrieved from https://sites.dartmouth.edu/dujs/2010/05/30/blindfolded-birdwatching-the-effect-of-harmonics-on-localization-of-bird-calls/

Boncoraglio, G. & N. Saino (2007). Habitat structure and the evolution of bird song: a meta-analysis of the evidence for the acoustic adaptation hypothesis. Functional Ecology 21(1): 134–142; https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2435.2006.01207.x

Brenowitz, E.A. (1982). The active space of red-winged blackbird song. Journal of Comparative Physiology 147(4): 511–522. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00612017

Brown, T.J. & P. Handford (2003). Why birds sing at dawn: the role of consistent song transmission. Ibis 145(1): 120–129. https://doi.org/10.1046/j.1474-919X.2003.00130.x

Brumm, H. (2006). Signalling through acoustic windows: nightingales avoid interspecific competition by short-term adjustment of song timing. Journal of Comparative Physiology A 192(12): 1279–1285. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00359-006-0158-x

Brumm, H. & H. Slabbekoorn (2005). Acoustic communication in noise. Advances in the Study of Behavior 35:151–209. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0065-3454(05)35004-2

Calder, W.A. (1990) The scaling of sound output and territory size: are they matched? Ecology 71: 1810–1816. https://doi.org/10.2307/1937589

Cardoso, G.C. (2010). Loudness of birdsong is related to the body size, syntax and phonology of passerine species. Journal of Evolutionary Biology 23(1): 212–219. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1420-9101.2009.01883.x

Cardoso, G.C. & J.W. Atwell (2011). On the relation between loudness and increased song frequency of urban birds. Animal Behaviour 82: 831–836. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2011.07.018

Cardoso, G.C. & P.G. Mota (2009). Loudness of syllables is related to syntax and phonology in the songs of canaries and seedeaters. Behaviour 146: 1649–1663.

Catchpole, C.K. & P.J.B. Slater (2008). Bird song: Biological themes and variations. 2nd ed., Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp. 203–239.

Cuthill, I.C. & W.A. Macdonald (1990). Experimental manipulation of the dawn and dusk chorus in the blackbird Turdus merula. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 26(3): 209–216. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00172088

Dabelsteen, T. & N. Mathevon (2002). Why do songbirds sing intensively at dawn? Acta Ethologica 4(2): 65–72; https://doi.org/10.1007/s10211-001-0056-8

Ey, E. & J. Fischer (2009). The “acoustic adaptation hypothesis” – a review of the evidence from birds, anurans and mammals. Bioacoustics 19(1–2): 21–48. https://doi.org/10.1080/09524622.2009.9753613

Ficken, R.W., M.S. Ficken & J.P. Hailman (1974). Temporal pattern shifts to avoid acoustic interference in singing birds. Science 183(4126): 762–763. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.183.4126.762

Fisher, R.A. (1925). Statistical Methods for Research Workers. Oliver and Boyd, Edinburgh, London, 145pp.

Forstmeier, W., B. Kempenaers, A. Meyer & B. Leisler (2002). A novel song parameter correlates with extra-pair paternity and reflects male longevity. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 269(1499), 1479-1485; https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2002.2039

Gage, S.H. & A.C. Axel (2014). Visualization of temporal change in soundscape power of a Michigan lake habitat over a 4-year period. Ecological Informatics 21: 100–109. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecoinf.2013.11.004  

Garamszegi, L.Z. & A.P. Møller (2004). Extrapair paternity and the evolution of bird song. Behavioral Ecology 15(3): 508–519. https://doi.org/10.1093/beheco/arh041

Hart, P.J., R. Hall, W. Ray, A. Beck & J. Zook (2015). Cicadas impact bird communication in a noisy tropical rainforest. Behavioral Ecology 26(3): 839–842. https://doi.org/10.1093/beheco/arv018

Hartley, R.S. & R.A. Suthers (1989). Airflow and pressure during canary song: direct evidence for mini-breaths. Journal of Comparative Physiology A 165(1): 15–26. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00613795

Henwood, K. & A. Fabrick (1979). A quantitative analysis of the dawn chorus: Temporal selection for communicatory optimization. The American Naturalist 114: 260–274. https://doi.org/10.1086/283473

Hildebrand, J.A. (2009). Anthropogenic and natural sources of ambient noise in the ocean. Marine Ecology Progress Series 395: 5–20. https://doi.org/10.3354/meps08353

Hutchinson, J.M. (2002). Two explanations of the dawn chorus compared: how monotonically changing light levels favour a short break from singing. Animal Behaviour 64(4): 527–539. https://doi.org/10.1006/anbe.2002.3091

Irwin, R.E. (1990). Directional sexual selection cannot explain variation in song repertoire size in the New World Blackbirds (Icterinae). Ethology 85: 212–224. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1439-0310.1990.tb00401.x

Joo, W. (2008). Environmental sounds as an ecological variable to understand dynamics of ecosystems. Master’s Thesis. Department of Zoology, Michigan State University, East Lansing, Michigan.

Joo, W., S.H. Gage & E.P. Kasten (2011). Analysis and interpretation of variability in soundscapes along an urban–rural gradient. Landscape and Urban Planning 103(3–4): 259–276. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.landurbplan.2011.08.001

Kacelnik, A. (1979). The foraging efficiency of great tits (Parus major L.) in relation to light intensity. Animal Behaviour 27: 237–241. https://doi.org/10.1016/0003-3472(79)90143-X

Kasten, E.P., S.H. Gage, J. Fox & W. Joo (2012). The remote environmental assessment laboratory’s acoustic library: An archive for studying soundscape ecology. Ecological Informatics 12: 50–67. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecoinf.2012.08.001

Klump, G. (1996). Bird communication in the noisy world, pp. 321–338. In: Kroodsma, D.E. & E.H. Miller (eds.). Ecology and Evolution of Acoustic Communication in Birds. Cornell University Press, Ithaca (NY), 587pp.

Krause, B. (1987). Bioacoustics, habitat ambience in ecological balance. Whole Earth Review, 57(Winter).

Krebs, J.R. & N.B. Davies (1981). An Introduction to Behavioural Ecology. Blackwell Scientific Publications, Oxford, UK, 420pp.

Kroodsma, D.E. (1977). Correlates of song organization among North American wrens. The American Naturalist 111(981): 995–1008. https://doi.org/10.1086/283228

Kruskal, W.H. & W.A. Wallis (1952). Use of ranks in one-criterion variance analysis. Journal of the American Statistical Association 47(260): 583–621.

Luther, D. (2009). The influence of the acoustic community on songs of birds in a neotropical rain forest. Behavioral Ecology 20(4): 864–871. https://doi.org/10.1093/beheco/arp074

Mace, R. (1986). The importance of female behaviour in the dawn chorus. Animal Behaviour 34: 621–622. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0003-3472(86)80139-7

Mace, R. (1987). The dawn chorus in the great tit Parus major is directly related to female fertility. Nature 330: 745–746. https://doi.org/10.1038/330745a0

Marten, K. & P. Marler (1977). Sound transmission and its significance for animal vocalization. I. Temperate habitats. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 2: 271–290. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00299740

Marten, K., D. Quine & P. Marler (1977). Sound transmission and its significance for animal vocalization. II Tropical forest habitats. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 2: 291–302. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00299741

McNamara, J.M., R.H. Mace & A.I. Houston (1987). Optimal daily routines of singing and foraging in a bird singing to attract a mate. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 20(6): 399–405. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00302982

Morton, E.S. (1975). Ecological sources of selection on avian sounds. The American Naturalist 109(965): 17–34; https://doi.org/10.1086/282971

Mukherjee, D. & S. Bhupathy (2007). A new species of wolf snake (Serpentes: Colubridae: Lycodon) from Anaikatti Hills, Western Ghats, Tamil Nadu, India. Russian Journal of Herpetology 14(1): 21–26.

Mullet, T.C. (2017). Connecting Soundscapes to Landscapes: Modeling the Spatial Distribution of Sound, pp. 211–224. In: Farina, A. & S.H. Gage (eds.). Ecoacoustics: The Ecological Role of Sounds. John Wiley and Sons, India, 336pp.

Mullet, T.C., S.H. Gage, J.M. Morton & F. Huettmann (2016). Temporal and spatial variation of a winter soundscape in south-central Alaska. Landscape Ecology 31(5): 1117–1137. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10980-015-0323-0

Napoletano, B.M. (2004). Measurement, quantification and interpretation of acoustic signals within an ecological context. MSc. Thesis. Department of Zoology, Michigan State University. Department of Zoology, xii+186.

Nelson, D.A. & P. Marler (1990). The perception of birdsong and an ecological concept of signal space, pp. 443–478. In: W.C. Stebbins & M.A. Berkley (eds.). Comparative perception, Vol. 2. Complex signals. John Wiley & Sons, England, 483pp.

Noss, R.F., W.J. Platt, B.A. Sorrie, A.S. Weakley, D.B. Means, J. Costanza & R.K. Peet (2015). How global biodiversity hotspots may go unrecognized: lessons from the North American Coastal Plain. Diversity and Distributions 21(2): 236–244. https://doi.org/10.1111/ddi.12278

Nowicki, S. & D.A. Nelson (1990). Defining Natural Categories in Acoustic Signals: Comparison of Three Methods Applied to ‘Chick-a-dee’ Call Notes. Ethology 86(2): 89–101. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1439-0310.1990.tb00421.x

Oberweger, K. & F. Goller (2001). The metabolic cost of birdsong production. Journal of Experimental Biology 204(19): 3379–3388.

Pijanowski, B.C., L.J. Villanueva-Rivera, S.L. Dumyahn, A. Farina, B.L. Krause, B.M. Napoletano, S.H. Gage & N. Pieretti (2011). Soundscape ecology: the science of sound in the landscape. BioScience 61(3): 203–216; https://doi.org/10.1525/bio.2011.61.3.6

Planque, R. & H. Slabbekoorn (2008). Spectral overlap in songs and temporal avoidance in a Peruvian bird assemblage. Ethology 114(3): 262–271. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1439-0310.2007.01461.x

Podos, J. (1997). A performance constraint on the evolution of trilled vocalizations in a songbird family (Passeriformes: Emberizidae). Evolution 51(2): 537–551. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1558-5646.1997.tb02441.x

Praveen J., R. Jayapal & A. Pittie (2019). Checklist of the birds of India (v3.0). Website: http://www.indianbirds.in/india/ [Date of publication: 05 May, 2019].

Prestwich, K.N. (1994). The energetics of acoustic signaling in anurans and insects. American Zoologist 34(6): 625–643. https://doi.org/10.1093/icb/34.6.625

Qi, J., S.H. Gage, W. Joo, B. Napoletano & S. Biswas (2008). Soundscape Characteristics of an Environment: A New Ecological Indicator of Ecosystem Health. Wetland and Water Resource Modeling and Assessment. CRC Press, New York, 655pp.

Singh, P. & T.D. Price (2015). Causes of the latitudinal gradient in birdsong complexity assessed from geographical variation within two Himalayan warbler species. Ibis 157(3): 511–527. https://doi.org/10.1111/ibi.12271

Slabbekoorn, H. (2004). Habitat-dependent ambient noise: consistent spectral profiles in two African forest types. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 116(6): 3727–3733. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.1811121

Slagsvold, T. (1996). Dawn and dusk singing of male American robins in relation to female behavior. The Wilson Bulletin 507–515. https://www.jstor.org/stable/4163717

Sony Linear PCM Recorder PCM-M10 (2009). Retrieved from Sony website. https://www.docs.sony.com/release/PCMM10.pdf

SPSS Inc (2007). SPSS for Windows, Rel.16.0.0. SPSS Inc, Chicago

Stanley, C.Q., M.H. Walter, M.X. Venkatraman & G.S. Wilkinson (2016). Insect noise avoidance in the dawn chorus of Neotropical birds. Animal Behaviour 112: 255–265. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2015.12.003

Sturkie, P.D. (Ed.) (1976). Avian Physiology. Springer-Verlag, New York, 400pp.

Sueur, J., S. Pavoine, O. Hamerlynck, & S. Duvail (2008). Rapid acoustic survey for biodiversity appraisal. Plos One 3(12): e4065. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0004065

Velásquez, N.A., F.N. Moreno-Gómez, E. Brunetti & M. Penna (2018). The acoustic adaptation hypothesis in a widely distributed South American frog: southernmost signals propagate better. Scientific Reports 8(1): 6990. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-018-25359-y

Wiley, R.H. & D.G. Richards (1978). Physical constraints on acoustic communication in the atmosphere: implications for the evolution of animal vocalizations. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 3(1): 69–94. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00300047

Wiley, R.H. & D.G. Richards (1982). Adaptations for acoustic communication in birds: sound propagation and signal detection pp. 131–181. In: Kroodsma, D.E. & E.H. Miller (eds.). Acoustic Communication in Birds, Vol. 1. Academic Press, New York, 371pp.

Wiley, R.H. (1991). Associations of song properties with habitats for territorial oscine birds of eastern North America. The American Naturalist 138: 973–993. https://doi.org/10.1086/285263