A note on the behaviour of Four-horned Antelope Tetracerus quadricornis de Blainville, 1816 (Mammalia: Cetartiodactyla: Bovidae) in lowland Nepal

 

Krishna Prasad Pokharel

 

Chair of Wildlife Ecology and Management, Faculty of Environment and Natural Resources, University of Freiburg, Germany

kp.pokharel@gmail.com

 

 

Abstract: Behavioural studies provide the reasons behind habitat preferences of animals and their fitness to survive and propagate. The Four-horned Antelope, an endangered endemic species to the Indian subcontinent was monitored at Ratamate area of Babai Valley in Bardia National Park, Nepal. We used ad libitum sampling and focal animal sampling within the rule for continuous recording of ‘all-occurrences’ of ‘vigilance’ behaviour. We found that the Four-horned Antelope remains ‘alert and vigilant’ during 40% of its behavioural time budget when it scans the surroundings with raised head, with or without chewing. In the event of sudden threat it ‘freezes’, lying down still and hiding in the ground cover. Therefore, maintenance of ground cover should form a regular practice in conservation management of the Four-horned Antelope.

 

Keywords: Bardia National Park, behaviour, ground cover requirement, Four-horned Antelope, Tetraceros quadricornis.

 

Abbreviations: FHA - Four-horned Antelope; BD - Barking Deer; IUCN - International Union for Nature Conservation; DHM - Department of Hydrology and Meteorology.

 

 

doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.11609/JoTT.o3985.7269-73

Editor: Mewa Singh, University of Mysore, Mysuru, India. Date of publication: 26 May 2015 (online & print)

 

Manuscript details: Ms # o3985 | Received 04 April 2014 | Final received 21 April 2015 | Finally accepted 27 April 2015

 

Citation: Pokharel, K.P. (2015). A note on the behaviour of Four-horned Antelope Tetracerus quadricornis de Blainville, 1816 (Mammalia: Cetartiodactyla: Bovidae) in lowland Nepal. Journal of Threatened Taxa 7(6): 7269–7273; http://dx.doi.org/10.11609/JoTT.o3985.7269-73

 

Copyright: © Pokharel 2015. Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License. JoTT allows unrestricted use of this article in any medium, reproduction and distribution by providing adequate credit to the authors and the source of publication.

 

Funding: KPP was supported by a PhD scholarship from state graduate funding (LGFG) through International Graduate Academy (IGA) of the University of Freiburg.

 

Competing Interest: The authors declare no competing interests.

 

Acknowledgements: I am thankful to the Department of National Parks and Wildlife Conservation (DNPWC) for research permits, and the Bardia National Park and National Trust for Nature Conservation (NTNC) for their logistical support. Thanks are also due to Liam Donnelly for manuscript language correction.

 

 

223732.jpg

 

 

Behavioural studies provide knowledge on habitat ecology of a species and help in conservation. Such studies inform us about the reasons behind the habitat preferences of an animal, their response to predators or threats, and breeding/survival success (Sutherland 1998; Caro 2007). The Four-horned Antelope Tetracerus quadricornis Blainville, 1816, also known as “Chausingha” or “Chauka”, is endemic to the Indian subcontinent. Very limited ecological information is available for this threatened species. According to the IUCN Red List of Threatened Species, this is a ‘Vulnerable’ species with a decreasing population trend (Mallon 2008) and considered ‘Data Deficient’ in Nepal (Jnawali et al. 2011). The Four-horned Antelope (FHA) is scattered through most of India (Rahmani 2001) and some pockets of lowland Nepal such as Chitwan and Bardia National Parks (Bolton 1975, 1976; Pokharel & Storch 2012; Pokharel et al. 2015) (Images 1–3). Because of limited ecological information the species has received less concern in the field of wildlife conservation in Nepal. With this background information, we conducted a field research in Babai Valley, Bardia National Park to explore the ‘alert’ or ‘vigilance behaviour’ of FHA with the aim of improving the existing knowledge on behavioural ecology and approach to conservation.

 

Materials and Methods

Study area

The present study was conducted at Ratamate area of Babai Valley in southeastern part of Bardia National Park (28028′26.5″N & 81029′10.1″E). The park is located in lowland Nepal covering an area of 968km2 near the Indian border, 390km west of Kathmandu. Monthly mean temperature of the area ranges from a minimum of 100C in January to a maximum of 450C in June. The park has a subtropical monsoonal climate with three distinct seasons; monsoon, winter and dry. The annual rainfall is 1,560–2,230 mm, most of which is recorded during the monsoon from June to September (DHM 2005–2009, unpublished data). During the dry season, from March till the end of May, air moisture level is very low and dry biomass is burnt every year to provide fresh pasture to herbivores. Fresh vegetation sprouts with the pre-monsoon rain during May indicates the end of the dry season. There is ample food for herbivores and easy access to water during the monsoon while the dry season is harsh because almost all the forest undergrowth gets burnt. Water is localised to rivers and certain gorges, and all seasonal streams dry up in summer. The Ratamate area provides a perennial water source and mineral licks. Therefore, many animals in Babai Valley visit Ratamate.

The vegetation in Babai Valley consists of Climax Sal Shorea robusta and mixed deciduous forest. A mosaic of riverine forest and early successional floodplain communities are along the Babai River and its tributaries. Patches of grassland, locally known as ‘phanta’, and tall grass at early succession stage are scattered throughout the Sal forest (Upreti 1994; Pokharel 2010). Chital Axis axis, Wild Boar Sus scrofa, Barking Deer Muntiacus vaginalis, and Sambar Rusa unicolor are some of the commonly found herbivores in the park whereas Tiger Panthera tigris, Leopard P. pardus, Sloth Bear Melursus ursinus, Striped Hyena Hyaena hyaena, and Dhole Cuon alpinus are the main predators (Heinen & Kattel 1992).

 

224481.jpg

 

 

Study design

FHAs are sighted in most of the vegetation associations available in Babai Valley but are not easy to monitor for long durations because of their shy nature, small body size and poor visibility inside the forest. Therefore, monitoring of FHA in relatively open areas such as stream banks where they come to drink water and at the sites of mineral licking was resorted to in this study. Areas with unobstructed line of vision such as the top of a cliff or a branch of a tree were preferentially used for making observations so that the animal could be seen clearly without being disturbed.

The methods of ad libitum sampling and focal animal sampling were used in accordance with Martin & Bateson (1993) within the ambit of rules for continuous- or “all-occurrence” recording. The objective was to have data that could provide exact and reliable record on behaviour by measuring true frequencies and durations of different activity patterns. Three different adult FHAs (two females and one male), who came to visit a mine licking site, were observed from 15–18 May 2010 for a total of approximately five hours from early morning to evening using a pair of binoculars (Pentax 10×42). Observations paused for 1–2 minutes after an observation for approximately 10mins and stopped when the focal animal was no longer visible. Sex and age group of focal animal were noted down and eight different activity patterns were identified. They include alert/vigilance (raising head above shoulder level while scanning the surrounding and includes both vigilance with and without chewing), looking at subject (looking at the researcher raising head above shoulder with constant body posture), freezing (hiding inside the bush without active body movement), walking (moving with head either below shoulder or at the level of shoulder and/or searching for food but not collecting), drinking (drinking water), licking (feeding on soil), browsing (collecting food/vegetative materials) and other activities (other activity patterns such as itching body, shaking tail while neither eating nor scanning the surrounding, and smelling).

 

224483.jpg

 

 

Data analysis

Only three individuals (two females and one male) could be observed in the field for behavioural study. Therefore behavioural patterns are expressed separately and could not be compared statistically between sexes due to insufficient replications. Furthermore, data on freezing behaviour was excluded from data analysis because the data could be collected for only one female as a single and sudden event. The data on alertness was transferred into frequency table to obtain the frequency of head raising per minute. Furthermore, percentage of time spent was used to compare the different activity patterns of three individuals, and was calculated as:

n

time spent = (xi / X) * 100

i=1

where xi = duration of an activity i; and X = total duration observed

 

 

Results and Discussion

Activity patterns

The female FHAs were observed for more time in the open area (88.10%) than the adult male (11.90%). Alert behaviour was observed to be occurring for most of the time (39.59%) followed by mineral licking (25.63%) and walking (21.28%) (Table 1; Fig. 1). Other behaviours occurred only with small durations. Females spent more time licking than the male whereas only the male was observed to browse. The dense riverine forest was within a distance of 100m. All focal individuals noticed the researcher and were quite alert at the beginning. The female FHAs seemed to be very aware of the researcher in comparison to the male FHA which did not pay much attention.

 

223543.jpg

 

 

Frequency of alertness/vigilance

The range of vigilance activity, in terms of number of head-uplift per minute, ranged from 0–6. Mean frequency of head raising among three individuals was found to be 2.06±1.206 (standard deviation of mean). During vigilance, the animal frequently raised its head and stopped all other activities for a while before getting back to foraging or licking activity. At the beginning, when it visited mine licking sites, it was more vigilant, but with time, it became less attentive and its head raising frequency decreased. However, when it noticed any disturbance, even the call of a bird, it became alert for a moment. Even though there was a relatively low level of inter-specific as well as intra-specific disturbance during field observation, food intake rate was limited by vigilant activity rather than by chewing and swallowing.

 

224219.jpg

 

 

Response of FHA to disturbance

Freezing behaviour: The “freezing behaviour” was observed only in an adult female and a young one. While two young FHAs were following their mother along the forest edge, a troop of Hanuman Langurs Semnopithecus entellus arrived and disturbed them. The adult female, the mother, did not respond to them, but one of the young FHA quickly ran to its mother and remained very close to her, but the other remained still, in the tall grass. After 2–3 minutes, the monkey troop moved away, and the young FHA came out and ran to its mother. They were observed for a very short time. When the mature female noticed the presence of two of our team members at a distance of about 150m, she moved silently to a nearby bush of tall grass, entered about 5m inside and sat down. Because of the dry season, the colour of ground vegetation i.e., grass, was similar to her body colour, therefore, it was very hard to see her inside the bush camouflage. She stayed there for about 32mins without moving from her position but she moved her pinna slowly to detect the potential danger and its direction.

Other type of response: In addition to their regular vigilance pattern, FHAs seemed to be quite attentive to every disturbance they perceived. They spent a considerable amount of time looking at the event or in the direction of the disturbance. The researcher remained motionless when the female FHAs looked at the researcher. After a period of time the female FHAs perceiving no threat shifted their activity to another event. The longest time that the female FHAs spent continuously looking at the researcher was 3 minutes 19 seconds. Even for such a long time, both FHAs did not look around for the other probable predation risk, neither did they try to run away. Later on, the same animal did not spend such amount of time even if they saw the researcher. The male FHA did not spend a long duration of time concentrated on the researcher as compared to the females.

During the field study, FHA responded more towards Chital and Barking deer (BD) than towards Macaques (Macaca sp.) FHA used to spend some moments being watchful when it saw Chital and even Chitals responded similarly, but FHA was more alert than the Chital. Evidently, the Chital drew its attention to FHA first. The same behaviour occurred when FHA and BD encountered each other. FHA was alert not only in the presence of other species, but also in the presence of other FHAs. When an adult male was active at a mine licking site and an adult female arrived, both of them remained alert when they noticed each other. After a while, the male drew its attention to the female allowing her to come to the mine licking site at the same place. Then the female slowly moved towards the male, they smelled each other and accepted the presence of each other. Then they started to perform the normal activity of licking, walking or drinking. Once, when an adult female was returning to the nearest forest, west bank of Ratamate Stream, she encountered a group of FHA with an adult male and a female (mother) with a young one. At that time, the mother FHA with her young offspring was more alert than the male. The young one did not care about the new comer. The adult female lowered her head, stretched her neck towards them (group of FHA) and smelled the air. When the group of FHAs, probably members of a family, did not react in an agonistic way, the adult FHA moved slowly towards the group lowering her head and shaking her tail frequently showing submissive behaviour. Later, all FHAs behaved in a normal way and formed a new group as if they were familiar with each other and started to forage. After a few minutes, they disappeared into the forest.

In conclusion, Four-horned Antelopes were observed to spend ca. 40% of their time being vigilant and were aware of every kind of disturbance they perceive. In response to threat they preferred to hide and freeze into the vegetation cover rather than try to escape. Therefore, conservation management in Bardia National Park and in other areas of Four-horned Antelope distribution should consider maintaining the ground cover.

 

 

References

 

Bolton, M. (1975 & 1976). Royal Chitwan National Park Management Plan, FAO/UNDP National Parks and Wildlife Conservation Project, Nepal. In: Heinen, J.T. & P.B. Yonzon (1994). A review of conservation issues and programs in Nepal: from a single species focus toward biodiversity protection. Mountain Research and Development 14(1): 61–76.

Caro, T. (2007). Behaviour and conservation: A bridge too far? Trends in Ecology and Evolution 22(8): 394–400; http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tree.2007.06.003

DHM (2005–2009). Climatological data of Bardia, Nepal. Department of Hydrology and Meteorology, Government of Nepal.

Heinen, J.T. & B. Kattel (1992). Parks, people and conservation: a review of management issues in Nepal’s protected areas. Population and Environment 14: 49–84; http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF01254607

Jnawali, S.R., H.S. Baral, S. Lee, K.P. Acharya, G.P. Upadhyay, M. Pandey, R. Shrestha, D. Joshi, B.R. Laminchhane, J. Griffiths, A.P. Khatiwada, N. Subedi & R. Amin (compilers) (2011). The Status of Nepal Mammals. The National Red List Series, Department of National Parks and Wildlife Conservation Kathmandu, Nepal, viii+266pp.

Mallon, D.P. (2008). Tetracerus quadricornis. In: IUCN (2010). IUCN Red List of Threatened Species. Version 2010.2. <www.iucnredlist.org>. Downloaded on 21 August 2010.

Martin, P. & P. Bateson (1993). Measuring behaviour, An Introductory Guide - 2nd Edition. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, UK.

Pokharel, K. & I. Storch (2012). Factors influencing the spatial distribution patterns of the Four-horned Antelope in lowland Nepal. Gnusletter 30(2): 13–14.

Pokharel, K.P. (2010). Factors influencing the spatial distribution patterns of the Four-Horned Antelope in Babai Valley, Bardia National Park, Nepal. MSc Thesis. Department of forest and environmental science, University of Freiburg, xxiii+47pp.

Pokharel, K.P., T. Ludwig & I. Storch (2015). Spatial niche partitioning in sub-tropical solitary ungulates: Four-Horned Antelope and Barking Deer in Nepal. PLoS ONE 10: e0117917; http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0117917

Rahmani, A.R. (2001). India, pp. 178–187. In: Mallon, D.P. & S.C. Kingswood (eds.). Antelopes Part 4: Global Survey and Regional Action Plans: North Africa, the Middle East, and Asia. Antelope Specialist Group, IUCN, Gland, Switzerland.

Sutherland, W.J. (1998). Importance of behavioural studies on conservation biology. Animal Behaviour 56: 801–809; http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/anbe.1998.0896

Upreti, B.N. (1994). In: Steinheim, G., P. Wegge, J.I. Fjellstad, S.R. Jnawali, & R. Weladji (2005). Dry season diets and habitat use of sympatric Asian Elephants (Elephas maximus) and Greater One-horned Rhinoceros (Rhinocerus unicornis) in Nepal. Journal of Zoology 265: 377–385; http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0952836905006448